SIGHTINGS



Selfish Animals? Scientists
Debunk The Idea Of
Animal Altruism
www.foxnews.com
By Amanda Onion
6-9-99
 
 
NEW YORK - In Charles Dickens' A Tale of Two Cities, Sydney Carton goes to the guillotine to spare the lover of the woman he loves.
 
"It is a far, far better thing that I do, than I have ever done," reflects the self-sacrificing Carton on the scaffolding.
 
Such acts of human altruism " found both in fiction and real life " defy the rules of Darwinism. By giving his life, Carton loses any chance for continuing his genetic line. It's behavior that has stumped biologists for centuries and inspired them to scour the animal kingdom for signs of similar behavior among other species.
 
Some say they have looked too hard.
 
"Many of my colleagues have really wanted to believe that animals are nice to each other," said David Stephens, a biologist at the University of Minnesota and one of the leading skeptics of animal altruism. "But often behavior that appears to be animal generosity turns out to be more selfish when you consider the economics more closely."
 
Looking Out for Number One
 
Most recently, scrutiny has been placed on the guarding behavior of the fuzzy, burrowing meerkat in South Africa. Scientists have long believed that individual meerkats act bravely and selflessly when they take turns watching out for predators while their peers forage for insects. When a guarding meerkat spies a predator it yelps a warning call and the entire group then darts for cover in the nearest burrows.
 
Not only do the sentinels sacrifice the chance to feed in order to guard the safety of the group, scientists also believed the creatures place themselves at higher risk by exposing themselves at high perches.
 
Not so, says Cambridge University's Tim Clutton-Brock. After more than 2,000 hours of observing five packs of meerkats, Clutton-Brock's group reported they did not see a single sentinel attacked or killed by a predator. That led them to conclude in the current issue of the journal Science, that individual meerkats that stand guard are actually looking out for their own skins.
 
"We found because these individuals were the first to see the predator, they were also the first ones to get below ground," Clutton-Brock said.
 
The behavior also couldn't be explained by kin selection " a common explanation of animal altruism that suggests animals sometimes place themselves at risk to protect their close relatives " and therefore their genes. Clutton-Brock found that everyone in a group, whether relatives or not, takes turns standing guard. What's more, he learned that a meerkat was only likely to stand guard if it had already fed enough to fill its belly.
 
The meerkat study is only the latest in a string of newly disproved cases of animal altruism. And Stephens argues the reason is clear. "Animals seem to act in their own self interest," he said. "No one should be surprised by that."
 
Strength in Numbers
 
One of Stephens' favorite examples are ravens who call their mates when they find a carcass. Scientists previously assumed these individuals were simply being generous. But a biologist at the University of Vermont recently pointed out that ravens only call their peers when the carcass they find is in another group's territory. By calling in their mates, the ravens beef up their defense against rival birds.
 
Swallows also demonstrate seemingly altruistic behavior when they alert other swallows about a newly-found swarm of insects. While it may appear the individual swallow is sharing its find by recruiting others to feed on the swarm, it turns out the bird is actually improving its chances of catching some dinner. Studies show the birds are more effective at tracking an insect swarm and nipping bugs out of the air when chasing them in a group.
 
"It's clearly not as simple as many imagined it was ten years ago," said Jerry Wilkinson, an animal behaviorist at the University of Maryland. "Now we are being forced to find alternative ways to explain these cooperative cases that don't fall neatly into kinship or reciprocal altruism."
 
Scratch My Back, I'll Scratch Yours
 
Still, there remain incredible cases of what biologists call reciprocal altruism, or you-scratch-my-back, I'll-scratch-yours behavior. While animals in these scenarios stand to gain in the long run by their behavior, they also sacrifice much personal fitness to help others in the meantime.
 
Male species of the tropical long-tailed manakin, for example, often team up in musical duos to serenade single females with elaborate melodies. Typically, one of the males never manages to mate with the female, but serves as the alpha male's celibate, musical sidekick for up to seven years.
 
The humble sidekick is not rewarded until the dominant " and usually older " alpha male dies and the younger bird takes over its territory. Among manakins, acquiring territory also mean acquiring mates, and the sidekick's extended abstinence finally comes to an end.
 
All For One
 
Perhaps most impressive among the kinds of animal altruism is behavior which hinders the individual, but advances the welfare of an entire group. Wilkinson has observed female vampire bats sharing blood with unrelated females in their roosts. Similarly, individual female desert ants bring back food to share with unrelated ants in their colony. Even meerkats, while they may act selfishly as sentries, demonstrate group altruism when females stay behind in burrows to baby-sit unrelated infants from their colonies.
 
Stephens believes further scrutiny into these cases may reveal selfish motives behind the behavior, but others claim there can be selfish reasons behind promoting a group.
 
"Natural selection can operate at the individual and group level," argued Lee Dugatkin, author of the new book, Cheating Monkeys and Citizen Bees. "Groups that have individuals who are willing to take on tasks that incur costs, do better than groups that do not have those individuals."
 
In fact, many have used group altruism to explain why humans are often so good to each other. By cooperating, some argue, people are only ensuring their own best interests as members of a society.
 
But while the group theory may explain soldiers sacrificing their lives to defend their country, or societies supporting a welfare system, it does not explain why Sydney Carton would lose his head for an unrequited love. Still, Carton met his death feeling noble in spirit. And, in some rare cases among emotionally-complex humans, that may be enough.
 
"People say they do things because it makes them 'feel good,'" said Dugatkin. "Is that a valid reason biologically? Science hasn't yet figured that one out."





SIGHTINGS HOMEPAGE